Science
‘Mindblowing’: how James Webb telescope’s snapshots of infant universe transformed astronomy


|
It took a remarkably long time for the light emitted by a group of ancient galaxies to reach the James Webb space telescope last year. Astronomers have calculated that the photons were in transit for more than 13bn years – almost the entire history of the cosmos – before they reached the orbiting observatory.
The results are scientifically dramatic and have revealed that the universe was already deep into the process of star formation only a short time after its big bang birth – although the photographs themselves are scarcely stunning in appearance: a handful of smudges, a couple of glowing spheres and an image that has been described as a glowing dog bone.
The world of astronomy has been dazzled, nevertheless. Among the objects caught in the telescope’s giant mirror is one that turns out to be the oldest known galaxy in the universe. The prosaically named JADES-GS-z13-0 appears as it did a mere 320m years after the big bang, long before the creation of our own planet. It also turns out to be tiny compared with our own galaxy, yet it was clearly creating new stars at a rate comparable to the Milky Way.
Intriguingly, this stellar fecundity is shared by several other ancient galaxies photographed by the James Webb telescope (JWST). These snapshots of the infant universe show that the first stars and galaxies had already formed and were evolving much earlier than most scientists had expected.
“These galaxies are very, very young yet they have already become hotbeds for star formation. It’s remarkable,” said Prof Brant Robertson, an astrophysicist at the University of California, Santa Cruz.
This enthusiasm was shared by Kevin Hainline, an astronomer at the University of Arizona, Tucson. “We have observed the earliest galaxies in the universe and it has been thrilling,” he told the Observer. “It has opened an entirely new chapter in the history of astronomy. It is telling us the universe was dynamic from the beginning.”
The £6.8bn James Webb telescope – the most ambitious, costly robot probe ever built – was launched on Christmas Day 2021, and took six months to position itself in deep space while its 18 hexagons of gold-coated beryllium mirror were unfurled and slotted together like a blooming flower to create a huge 6.5-metre (21ft) mirror. Then, exactly a year ago, the James Webb began taking its first images of the cosmos.
The completion of its first year of operations was celebrated last week by Nasa who built the observatory with European and Canadian space agency collaboration. The US space agency released images which depicted stars in our own galaxy that were coalescing out of clouds of interstellar dust. Given that it had taken more than 30 years to design and build the James Webb telescope – which endured major delays and threats of cancellation throughout its history – the anniversary was always going to be marked as an occasion that mixed relief with spectacle.
And the photographs of the Rho Ophiuchi star field are certainly stunning. However, the far more muted images of JADES-GS-z13-0 and its ancient partner galaxies are causing real excitement among cosmologists and astrophysicists.
More than any other set of observations, they underline the true potential of the James Webb telescope, it is argued.
“What is so surprising is the detail of the early universe that it has revealed,” said Sandro Taccella, a Cambridge University astrophysicist. “Theory predicted that very complex cosmological processes would be occurring at this time, though I did not expect to be able to observe them. However, the telescope takes such magnificently sharp images, we can actually see this complexity in operation. It was surprising – and very gratifying.”
In the first moments after the big bang, the universe was extremely hot and dense. As it cooled, protons and neutrons combined to form atomic nuclei which – after a few hundred thousand years – trapped electrons to create the first atoms. These coalesced into clouds of gas from which the first stars emerged hundreds of millions of years later.
However, another type of matter also emerged from the big bang, one that accounts for a very large fraction of all the mass in the universe. This is called dark matter and is only known through its gravitational effects – which were considerable, it transpires. “Dark matter assembled first after the big bang and began creating halos of unseen material which then attracted hydrogen and helium atoms to create gas clouds from which stars and galaxies eventually formed,” said Taccella.
“If it hadn’t been for dark matter, stars and galaxies would not have appeared until much later in the universe’s history. Now we have the James Webb, we can study how that happened in detail and hopefully get a better understanding of the role of dark matter in shaping the cosmos.”
The fact that dark matter played a key role in greatly speeding up the formation of the first stars and galaxies is highlighted through the photographs taken of ancient galaxies that include JADES-GS-z13-0. “It is already a complex structure, and that is mindblowing,” said Hainline. “We can see that it is a growing galaxy and that is a really beautiful thing.”
after newsletter promotion


Only a handful of ancient galaxies had been discovered before the launch of the James Webb telescope. Using the observatory, the project known as JADES – the JSWT Advanced Deep Extragalactic Survey – has identified a total of 717 such objects in less than a year.
“The really interesting question is what then happened to these galaxies,” added Robertson. “It is clear that they did not stay that way for the next 13bn years, but merged with other galaxies over time. That is how gravity works its wonders. It pulled them together so that they became bigger and bigger galaxies. And that is what you see in our own Milky Way today.
“We can actually see the remnants of other galaxies that were pulled in and accreted into our own galaxy. The images from the James Webb are showing us how that process began.”
How it all works
Designed as a replacement for the Hubble space telescope – launched in 1990 and still in operation – the James Webb is a far bigger, much more complex instrument with many more ambitious goals. These include studying the early universe, pinpointing possible life-supporting planets, and understanding how stars form.


However, to carry out these tasks, the observatory avoids using the visible part of the electromagnetic spectrum – unlike the Hubble and most ground-based telescopes – and instead gathers only infrared radiation. This is better for peering through dust and observing stars and planets as they coalesce out of clouds of gas and other material. In addition, the atmospheres of planets that contain chemicals such as methane – a gas associated with biological processes – are also best studied by using infrared radiation.
But most important of all is the role of infrared detectors in revealing the secrets of the early universe. Light gets fainter and redder the further back you look into the cosmos until its wavelength reaches the infrared part of the spectrum. You therefore need an infrared telescope if you want to study how the first stars, black holes and galaxies formed.
Operating infrared detectors is not easy, however. The James Webb telescope has to function at about 40C above absolute zero (about -233C) so that its instruments do not generate spurious heat signals that could swamp the faint infrared radiation it receives from the other side of the universe.
Far away from its warm home planet, the telescope is protected by a five-layer-thick shield that blocks out radiation from the sun and Earth while its instruments are chilled by a liquid helium refrigerator that is expected to keep it cool and operational for more than a decade.





Science
Sea ice around Antarctica recedes to historically low levels: Scientists


|
By clicking “Accept All Cookies”, you agree to the storing of cookies on your device and the processing of information obtained via those cookies (including about your preferences, device and online activity) by us and our commercial partners to enhance site navigation, personalise ads, analyze site usage, and assist in our marketing efforts. More information can be found in our
and . You can amend your cookie settings to reject non-essential cookies by clicking Cookie Settings below.





Science
Soyuz docks at International Space Station with two Russians, one American
|
A Russian spacecraft carrying two Russians and an American docked at the International Space Station on Friday after blasting off from Kazakhstan.
NASA astronaut Loral O’Hara and Roscosmos cosmonauts Oleg Kononenko and Nikolai Chub departed from the Baikonur Cosmodrome in Kazakhstan and docked at the ISS approximately three hours later.
Per NASA, the trio will join the station’s Expedition 69 crew comprising astronauts from the U.S., Russia, Denmark, and Japan. O’Hara will spend six months there while Kononenko and Chub will spend a year.
The trio was supposed to fly to the space station earlier this year, but their original capsule, Soyuz MS-23, was needed as a replacement for another crew. That crew — also two Russians and an American — will ride it home on September 27. Their stay was extended from six months to a year when the capsule developed a coolant leak while parked at the station.
It’s the first spaceflight for O’Hara and Chub, while mission commander Kononenko is on his fifth trip to the orbiting outpost. By the end of his yearlong stay, Kononenko will set a new record for the longest time in space, more than a thousand days.
Original article source: Soyuz docks at International Space Station with two Russians, one American





Science
Exploring the effect of pain on response to reward loss in calves
|
Abstract
Negative emotional states are known to interact, potentially aggravating one another. In this study, we used a well validated paradigm (successive negative contrast, SNC) to determine if pain from a common procedure (disbudding) influences responses to a reward downshift. Holstein calves (n = 30) were trained to approach a 0.5 L milk reward. Latency to approach, number of vocalisations and pressure applied on the bottle were recorded during training. To assess how pain affected responses to reward downshift, calves were randomly assigned to one of three treatments before the downshift. Two groups were disbudded and provided the ‘gold standard’ of pain mitigation: intraoperative local anesthesia and analgesia. One of these disbudded groups was then provided supplemental analgesic before testing. The third group was sham disbudded. All calves were then subjected to the reward downshift by reducing the milk reward to just 0.1 L. Interactions were detected between test session and daily trials on pressure applied for the Disbudded group (estimate ± SEM: 0.08 ± 0.05), and on vocalisations for the Sham (0.3 ± 0.1) and Disbudding + Analgesia (0.4 ± 0.1) groups. Our results indicate that SNC is a promising paradigm for measuring negative affect in calves and suggests that pain potentially affects the response to a reward downshift.
Introduction
A large body of research, primarily on rodents, has shown that sudden declines in reward levels are highly salient and provoke a negative affective response consistent with feelings of frustration or disappointment1,2. A well-developed paradigm for provoking this response is the successive negative contrast (SNC) test, where animals learn to obtain feed rewards which are then reduced in quantity or quality. Multiple lines of evidence indicate that this experience is distressing for animals, including increased levels of physiological markers of stress in rats and pigs3,4,5, and development of a preference for anxiolytic medication in rats6. In addition, responses to SNC are aggravated when an animal is in a pre-existing negative emotional state at the time of the test. For example, rats bred to be more anxious had higher latencies to approach a reward after a downshift7, and rats in amphetamine withdrawal displayed greater and longer reductions in reward consumption following a downshift8.
The influence of current affective state on SNC responses provides a compelling opportunity for the assessment of animal welfare, although only a handful of studies have employed this approach. In one study, rats housed in barren environments showed an extended increase in latency to approach the downshifted reward in comparison to rats housed in enriched environments, suggesting that these animals were more sensitive to reward loss than were rats in enriched housing9. Housing conditions (barren vs. enriched) also affected pigs’ sensitivity to reward loss10. To our knowledge, SNC has not been used to assess the emotional impact of pain in any species.
Research conducted on humans report that patients in a negative emotional state are more inclined to show anger responses11,12, and that pain can aggravate frustrating situations13.
In this study we tested if pain aggravates responses to SNC testing. Young cattle experience pain associated with routine farm procedures including hot-iron disbudding, indicated by physiological, behavioral and emotional responses to the procedure14,15,16,17,18. In this study we assessed the responses to SNC (in this case reducing the amount of milk available) in calves for three days following disbudding. Although providing a combination of local anesthesia and analgesia is considered a gold-standard in pain mitigation following disbudding, the duration of pain control has been challenging to estimate18, and disbudding pain has been suggested to last for several days19,20,21. For ethical reasons, all disbudded calves were provided local anesthesia and analgesia at the time of the procedure. To explore the potential longer-term pain caused by disbudding, a group of calves were provided additional fast-acting analgesia before tests. We predicted that calves in pain would respond to the downshift by increased pressure applied on the bottle containing the milk, number of vocalisations and latency to approach the reward. By exploring a novel approach to assessing the affective component of pain in animals, we hope to further the understanding of the emotional impact of a common farm procedure and, more generally, how negative states can interact to influence animal welfare.
Results
Maximum pressure applied to the reward bottle decreased across test days (− 0.4, t = − 3.5, P = 0.005) and daily trials (− 0.3, t = − 2.6, P = 0.01) (Fig. 1A). We also noted an interaction between test day and daily trial for the Disbudded group (0.1, t = 2.0, P = 0.05), and a tendency for the Sham group (0.08, t = 1.7, P = 0.09). There was no evidence of an interaction for pressure in the Disbudding + Analgesia group (0.07, t = 1.4, P = 0.2). Calves produced fewer vocalisations across days (− 1.5, t = − 4.6, P < 0.001) and daily trials (− 0.8, t = − 3.6, P < 0.001) (Fig. 1B). However, there were positive interactions between test day and trials for the Sham and Disbudding + Analgesia groups (0.3, t = 2.0, P = 0.05; 0.4, t = 2.7, P < 0.001 respectively). No interaction was found for the Disbudding group (0.05, t = 0.3, P = 0.7). Calves took longer to approach the reward across daily trials (0.4, t = 3.1, P = 0.002), with no effect of the test day (0.12, t = 0.9, P = 0.4) (Fig. 1C). Calves from the Sham group tended to decrease their latency across test day and daily trial (− 0.11, t = − 1.7, P = 0.09), whereas no interaction was found for the Disbudding (− 0.02, t = − 0.3, P = 0.8) and Disbudding + Analgesia groups (− 0.06, t = − 0.9, P = 0.3).
Calf responses to a reward downshift (from 0.5 L of milk to 0.1 L) over 3 test days, with 3 trials each day. An algometer was mounted behind the bottle containing the reward, measuring the maximum pressure exerted on the bottle by the calf (A). Vocalisations (B) and latency to approach (C) were also recorded. 24 h before the first test, calves were disbudded (Disbudding, Disbudding + Analgesia) or received a sham disbudding (Sham). One hour prior to tests, calves from the Disbudding + Analgesia group were administered an additional fast acting NSAID. Values presented are back transformed predicted values from mixed models. Colored circles represent mean estimates.
Discussion
After the reward downshift, calves responded by high pressure applied to the bottle and vocalisations. As calves went through more test sessions (with the lower reward), vocalisations and pressure both decreased, suggesting calves updated their expectations over time. Following the downshift, calves also increased their approach latency across trials within daily test sessions. This result is consistent with previous reports noting that approach latency increased after reward downshift 9,22.
We found some indication of a treatment effect on responses to the downshift over test days and daily trials. The significant positive interaction in pressure applied on the bottle over days and trials for the Disbudding group suggests some level of maintained frustration over tests. Similarly, only calves who had not been disbudded tended to decrease their approach latency across trials whereas calves from the Disbudding and Disbudding + Analgesia groups maintained their latency increase. This result is consistent with results from Burman and colleagues9 who reported a more prolonged response to reward downshift (i.e. higher latency) from rats assumed to be in a more negative affective state. This result is also consistent with work on calves showing increased anticipatory behavior in response to a reward downshift for animals housed in a more barren environment23. We had expected that calves receiving supplemental ketoprofen before the daily test sessions would have responded similarly to the sham calves. That these animals appeared to have some similar responses to the other disbudded calves suggests that our ketoprofen treatment protocol might not have mitigated the pain associated with disbudding during tests. Ketoprofen has been noted as appropriate pain control for disbudding24,25,26,27, but conflicting results have also been reported28,29.
In a study on SNC in chickens, Davies and colleagues30 found a gradual increase in approach latency and an immediate response in consummatory behaviours. They noted the gradual increase to be consistent with Thorndike’s law of effect31, analogous to an extinction mechanism where a less valuable reward induces a less ‘enthusiastic’ response over time. The disparity with consummatory responses was suggested to relate to the different timeframes of the measures: anticipatory responses such as approach latency might require conditioned learning, and therefore change more slowly. However, consummatory responses such as pressure applied are immediate indicators of reward evaluation, and therefore do not require an adjustment delay.
Contrary to our predictions, calves from the Disbudding group did not vocalize more than the other treatment groups after the downshift. These results remain unclear to us, but the very low number of vocalisations past the first test day questions the sensitivity of calves vocalisations counts when used in SNC paradigms.
The high variability among calves in their response to the downshift could be associated to intrinsic individual differences. Individual differences in traits such as fearfulness have been linked to pessimistic responses to a judgment bias test32. Moreover, such pessimism was also linked to the anhedonic response (i.e. a decrease in interest in a consummatory reward) following hot-iron disbudding33. Calves’ individual differences could also be dependent on the severity of the sensitization of their head caused by the procedure34,35, causing increased pain when coming into contact with the bottle. Alternatively, sucking on the nipple (even without milk) may be positive for calves36 and may also provide pain relief in the hours after disbudding37.
Conclusion
Following a reduction in a milk reward, calves who experienced a painful procedure appeared to potentially display an extended response to the downshift. Although SNC seems a promising avenue, our results remain tentative and further development of the paradigm and its applications must be investigated to identify its relevance to animal welfare assessment.
Methods
Ethics statement
Procedures were approved by The University of British Columbia Animal Care Committee under application A21-0111 and conducted in accordance with guidelines form the Canadian Council of Animal Care38. Reporting followed ARRIVE guidelines.
Animals and housing
The study was conducted at The University of British Columbia’s Dairy Education and Research Centre. To our knowledge, no study has used a similar paradigm in calves. To establish a sample size estimate, we relied on welfare studies using analogous SNC paradigms but applied to other species: rats (six subject per treatment9) and pigs (sixteen subjects per treatment group10). Considering this range and our own practical limitations, we settled on a sample size of ten subjects per treatment group. Thirty-five Holstein calves (all females) were initially enrolled in the study. Five calves were removed from the trial: three fell ill (scours and fever), one showed an extreme stress response when moved outside of her home pen, and one was not feed-restricted before a test. The thirty remaining had an average (± SD) birth weight of 38.3 ± 4.1 kg and were enrolled at 39.9 ± 4.1 d of age.
As routine farm practice, calves from all three treatments were intermingled in indoor pens (4.9 × 7.3 m, bedded with sawdust, and each containing eight to ten calves). Calves were provided ad libitum access to water and hay (RIC; Insentec B.V., Netherlands), and time-restricted access to 12 L of whole milk through a nipple feeder (CF 1000 CS Combi; DeLaval Inc., Sweden). To avoid long delay during trials, small replicates (average number of subjects per replicate = 3.5) were conducted.
Apparatus
The experimental apparatus was located in the same barn as the calves’ home pen, approximately 10–30 m away. The apparatus was a 1.8 × 1.2 m start-box leading to a 3.6 × 2.4 m pen through a vertical gate (Fig. 2A). Directly across from the start-box was a bottle and rubber teat mounted on rails, with an algometer (FPX 25, Wagner, Greenwich, USA) installed behind the bottle allowing measures of the maximum pressure applied to the bottle (Fig. 2B).
Calves were brought to the start-box, the vertical gate was lifted, and calves could access a milk reward (0.5 L during training, 0.1 L during tests) in the test pen (A). The bottle containing the milk reward was mounted on rails with an algometer positioned behind the bottle to measure the maximum pressure applied by the calf (B). Illustrations by Ann Sanderson.
Training
The trial was divided in three phases over seven days: training (three days), treatment (one day) and testing (three days). During training, calves were feed-restricted overnight (from 22:00 h) to ensure a high motivation for milk rewards over repeated trials. At approximately 10:00 h calves were individually brought into the apparatus, with no set order, and then placed in the start-box. The vertical gate was lifted and calves could approach and drink a 0.5 L milk reward from the bottle (this amount was based on previous studies on motivation trade-offs studies in calves37,39). Latency to contact the bottle (with mouth or tongue), latency to finish the reward, number of vocalisations and maximum pressure applied to the bottle were recorded live. The calf was then brought back to the start-box, the bottle refilled, and two more trials were conducted (i.e., for a total of three trials/d). After these trials were completed, the calf was returned to her home pen with full access to her daily milk allowance of (12 L/d). Training took place over three consecutive days, for a total of nine training trials. During the first day of training (for all three trials), no cues were given to the calf for the first minute after opening the start-box gate. After one minute, auditory (calls/whistle) and tactile (finger suckling) cues were given from the experimenter from outside the test-pen to get the calf’s attention towards the bottle. If these cues had failed after an additional minute, the experimenter would go inside the test pen and lead the calf to the bottle.
During the second and third day of training, no cues were given. If the calf had not approached the bottle within two minutes, the trial was recorded as a no-approach (and a pressure of zero applied to the bottle). Once a calf had approached the bottle, she had three additional minutes to finish the reward.
Treatments
Calves were pseudo-randomly assigned to one of three treatments (Disbudding, Disbudding + Analgesia, or Sham; ten calves each). Treatment assignment was balanced for age and birthweight (Disbudding: 40.7 ± 4.3 d, 38.7 ± 3.9 kg; Disbudding + Analgesia: 39.2 ± 7.0 d, 38.0 ± 5.6 kg; Sham: 39.7 ± 6.0 d, 38.3 ± 2.4 kg). On treatment day, calves were not feed-restricted and went through their treatment in their group pen at approximately 10:00 h. Regardless of treatment, calves were weighted and administered a multimodal pain mitigation strategy of sedative, local anesthesia and analgesia. The sedative was used to facilitate following injections and disbudding (xylazine 0.2 mg/kg Subcutaneous, Rompun 20 mg/mL, Bayer, Leverkusen, Germany). After sedation was reached (recumbency and eye rotation, approximately 10 min), a local anesthetic was injected as a cornual nerve block to mitigate the acute pain of the procedure (5 mL per side, lidocaine 2%, epinephrine 1:100,000, Lido-2, Rafter8, Calgary, AB, Canada), an NSAID was provided to minimize inflammation (meloxicam 0.5 mg/kg Subcutaneous, Metacam 20 mg/mL, Boehringer Ingelheim, Burlington, ON, Canada), and the horn bud area was shaved with an electric trimmer. Ten minutes after lidocaine injection, a pinprick test was done on the horn buds to test for pain reflex. For calves in the Disbudding and Disbudding + Analgesia treatments, a pre-heated electric dehorner (X30, 1.3 cm tip, Rhinehart, Spencerville, IN, USA) was applied to both horn buds until a consistent dark ring formed around each bud (requiring approximately 10 to 15 s). Calves from the Sham group were treated identically but instead of being disbudded, only pressure on the horn buds was applied with the plastic handle of the dehorner. After the procedure was completed, calves were positioned in sternal recumbency and left to recover in the pen. As the magnitude and duration of NSAID effects following disbudding remain unclear 18, calves from the Disbudding + Analgesia group received an additional NSAID injection (ketoprofen, 3 mg/kg, Subcutaneous, Anafen, 100 mg/mL, Boehringer Ingelheim, Ontario, Canada) 1 h before each of the three test sessions to provide supplemental pain control at the time of testing. Based on a previous study on the efficacy of ketoprofen after disbudding29, we expected ketoprofen to provide analgesic effects for up to 2 h following treatment.
Tests
In the three days following treatment, calves were tested for sensitivity to reward loss. Tests were similar to training: calves were brought individually to the apparatus after overnight feed restriction, and allowed access to a milk reward three times in a row (for a total of nine trials), but during testing the reward was reduced to 0.1 L. The time allowed for calves to approach and drink the reward was matched with their performance during training. Maximum pressure applied to the bottle, number of vocalisations and latency to approach were recorded. Calves from the Disbudding + Analgesia group received an additional NSAID injection (ketoprofen, 3 mg/kg, Subcutaneous, Anafen, 100 mg/mL, Boehringer Ingelheim, Ontario, Canada) 1 h before each of the three test sessions. After each session calves were returned to their home pen and again provided access to their full milk allowance (12 L). After the three test days calves were returned to routine farm care.
Statistical analysis
A mixed model was conducted on each outcome (maximum pressure, vocalisations and approach latency) on test phases (post treatment) using R’s lme4 package40. For pressure and latency, data were log transformed to fit model assumptions of linearity, normality and homoscedasticity. For vocalisation counts, we used a Poisson mixed model. Fixed factors were treatment (2 df), test day (1 df), daily trial (1 df) and their interaction (3 df). Daily trial, nested within day and Calf ID, was included as a random factor. Significance and tendency thresholds were set at P ≤ 0.05 and P ≤ 0.10, respectively. Data (Supplementary Information 1) and R code (Supplementary Information 2) are available in supplementary materials.
Data availability
The dataset and R code are freely available in supplementary materials.
References
-
Papini, M. R. Role of surprising nonreward in associative learning. Japanese J. Anim. Psychol. 56, 35–54 (2006).
-
Papini, M. R. Diversity of adjustments to reward downshifts in vertebrates. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 27, 420–445 (2014).
-
Dantzer, R., Arnone, M. & Mormede, P. Effects of frustration on behaviour and plasma corticosteroid levels in pigs. Physiol. Behav. 24, 1–4 (1980).
-
Flaherty, C. F., Becker, H. C. & Pohorecky, L. Correlation of corticosterone elevation and negative contrast varies as a function of postshift day. Anim. Learn. Behav. 13, 309–314 (1985).
-
Mitchell, C. & Flaherty, C. Temporal dynamics of corticosterone elevation in successive negative contrast. Physiol. Behav. 64, 287–292 (1998).
-
Manzo, L., Donaire, R., Sabariego, M., Papini, M. R. & Torres, C. Anti-anxiety self-medication in rats: Oral consumption of chlordiazepoxide and ethanol after reward devaluation. Behav. Brain Res. 278, 90–97 (2015).
-
Cuenya, L. et al. The effect of partial reinforcement on instrumental successive negative contrast in inbred Roman High- (RHA-I) and Low- (RLA-I) Avoidance rats. Physiol. Behav. 105, 1112–1116 (2012).
-
Barr, A. M. & Phillips, A. G. Increased successive negative contrast in rats withdrawn from an escalating-dose schedule of d-amphetamine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 71, 293–299 (2002).
-
Burman, O. H. P., Parker, R. M. A., Paul, E. S. & Mendl, M. Sensitivity to reward loss as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare. Biol. Lett. 4, 330–333 (2008).
-
Luo, L. et al. Effects of early life and current housing on sensitivity to reward loss in a successive negative contrast test in pigs. Anim. Cogn. 23, 121–130 (2020).
-
Fava, M. & Rosenbaum, J. F. Anger attacks in depression. Depress. Anxiety 8, 59–63 (1998).
-
Painuly, N., Sharan, P. & Mattoo, S. K. Relationship of anger and anger attacks with depression. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 255, 215–222 (2005).
-
Dow, C. M., Roche, P. A. & Ziebland, S. Talk of frustration in the narratives of people with chronic pain. Chronic Illn. 8, 176–191 (2012).
-
Adcock, S. J. J. & Tucker, C. B. Conditioned place preference reveals ongoing pain in calves 3 weeks after disbudding. Sci. Rep. 10, 3849 (2020).
-
Ede, T., Lecorps, B., Keyserlingk, M. A. G. & Weary, D. M. Calf aversion to hot-iron disbudding. Sci. Rep. 9, 5344 (2019).
-
Lecorps, B., Ludwig, B. R., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G. & Weary, D. M. Pain-induced pessimism and anhedonia: Evidence from a novel probability-based judgment bias test. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13, 54 (2019).
-
Neave, H. W., Daros, R. R., Costa, J. H. C., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G. & Weary, D. M. Pain and pessimism: Dairy calves exhibit negative judgement bias following hot-iron disbudding. PLoS ONE 8, e80556 (2013).
-
Winder, C. B. et al. Effects of local anesthetic or systemic analgesia on pain associated with cautery disbudding in calves: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Dairy Sci. https://doi.org/10.3168/jds.2017-14092 (2018).
-
Allen, K. A. et al. The effect of timing of oral meloxicam administration on physiological responses in calves after cautery dehorning with local anesthesia. J. Dairy Sci. 96, 5194–5205 (2013).
-
Heinrich, A., Duffield, T. F., Lissemore, K. D. & Millman, S. T. The effect of meloxicam on behavior and pain sensitivity of dairy calves following cautery dehorning with a local anesthetic. J. Dairy Sci. 93, 2450–2457 (2010).
-
Mintline, E. M. et al. Play behavior as an indicator of animal welfare: Disbudding in dairy calves. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 144, 22–30 (2013).
-
Rosas, J. M. et al. Successive negative contrast effect in instrumental runway behaviour: A study with Roman high- (RHA) and Roman low- (RLA) avoidance rats. Behav. Brain Res. 185, 1–8 (2007).
-
Neave, H. W., Webster, J. R. & Zobel, G. Anticipatory behaviour as an indicator of the welfare of dairy calves in different housing environments. PLoS ONE 16, e0245742 (2021).
-
Faulkner, P. M. & Weary, D. M. Reducing pain after dehorning in dairy calves. J. Dairy Sci. 83, 2037–2041 (2000).
-
Duffield, T. F. et al. Reduction in pain response by combined use of local lidocaine anesthesia and systemic ketoprofen in dairy calves dehorned by heat cauterization. Can. Vet. J. 51, 283–288 (2010).
-
Milligan, B. N., Duffield, T. & Lissemore, K. The utility of ketoprofen for alleviating pain following dehorning in young dairy calves. Can. Vet. J. 45, 140–143 (2004).
-
McMeekan, C. M. et al. Effects of regional analgesia and/or a non-steroidal anti-inflammatory analgesic on the acute cortisol response to dehorning in calves. Res. Vet. Sci. 64, 147–150 (1998).
-
Ede, T., Keyserlingk, M. A. G. & Weary, D. M. Assessing the affective component of pain, and the efficacy of pain control, using conditioned place aversion in calves. Biol. Lett. 15, 20190642 (2019).
-
Espinoza, C. A. et al. Evaluating the efficacy of a topical anaesthetic formulation and ketoprofen, alone and in combination, on the pain sensitivity of dehorning wounds in Holstein-Friesian calves. Anim. Prod. Sci. 56, 1512–1519 (2015).
-
Davies, A. C., Nicol, C. J. & Radford, A. N. Effect of reward downshift on the behaviour and physiology of chickens. Anim. Behav. 105, 21–28 (2015).
-
Thorndike, E. L. Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. viii, 297 (Macmillan Press, 1911). doi:https://doi.org/10.5962/bhl.title.55072.
-
Lecorps, B., Weary, D. M. & von Keyserlingk, M. A. G. Pessimism and fearfulness in dairy calves. Sci. Rep. 8, 1421 (2018).
-
Lecorps, B., Nogues, E., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G. & Weary, D. M. Pessimistic dairy calves are more vulnerable to pain-induced anhedonia. PLoS ONE 15, e0242100 (2020).
-
Casoni, D., Mirra, A., Suter, M. R., Gutzwiller, A. & Spadavecchia, C. Can disbudding of calves (one versus four weeks of age) induce chronic pain?. Physiol. Behav. 199, 47–55 (2019).
-
Mirra, A., Spadavecchia, C., Bruckmaier, R., Gutzwiller, A. & Casoni, D. Acute pain and peripheral sensitization following cautery disbudding in 1- and 4-week-old calves. Physiol. Behav. 184, 248–260 (2018).
-
Veissier, I. et al. Does nutritive and non-nutritive sucking reduce other oral behaviors and stimulate rest in calves?. J. Anim. Sci. 80, 2574–2587 (2002).
-
Adcock, S. J. J. & Tucker, C. B. Injury alters motivational trade-offs in calves during the healing period. Sci. Rep. 11, 6888 (2021).
-
Canadian Council on Animal Care. CCAC guidelines on: the care and use of farm animals in research, teaching and testing. (2009).
-
Ede, T., von Keyserlingk, M. A. G. & Weary, D. M. Approach-aversion in calves following injections. Sci. Rep. 8, 9443 (2018).
-
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Soft. 67, 1–48 (2015).
Acknowledgements
We thank the staff and students at The UBC Dairy Education and Research Centre for their help and support. We are especially grateful for the help provided by (alphabetical order) Joseph Lee, Kathen Lee, Emeline Nogues, Zimbabwe Osorio, Yasamin (Yas) Ranjbar, Russell Tucker, Raphaela Woodroffe and Emily Yau. This research was funded by a Discovery Grant (RGPIN-2016-0462) awarded to DMW from the National Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Ede, T., von Keyserlingk, M.A.G. & Weary, D.M. Exploring the effect of pain on response to reward loss in calves.
Sci Rep 13, 15403 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-42740-8
- Received: 05 December 2022
- Accepted: 14 September 2023
- Published: 16 September 2023
- DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-42740-8





-
News16 hours ago
India-Canada news: How the visa office suspension affects travellers
-
News17 hours ago
Why is rent going up faster in Brampton than everywhere else in Canada?
-
Business19 hours ago
Vote “No” to Unifor’s sellout Ford Canada contract! Build rank-and-File committees to fight for a North America-wide strike against the Detroit Three!
-
News15 hours ago
India-Canada news LIVE updates: Justin Trudeau says evidence was shared ‘many weeks ago’
-
News14 hours ago
Canada is still processing visas for Indian nationals
-
Art22 hours ago
Opera House totem pole permanently removed from city’s art collection
-
Art22 hours ago
Can David Salle Teach A.I. How to Create Good Art?
-
Economy19 hours ago
Weak Euro-Area PMI Data Suggest Economy Facing Contraction